References: Sensory Theory of Protolanguage

Ahlner, F., & Zlatev, J. (2010). Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism. Sign Systems Studies, 38(1), 298-348.

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660.

Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarström, H., Stadler, P. F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201605782.

Brang, D., Rouw, R., Ramachandran, V. S., & Coulson, S. (2011). Similarly shaped letters evoke similar colors in grapheme–color synesthesia. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1355-1358.

Brown, R., Black, A., & Horowitz, A. (1955). Phonetic symbolism in natural languages. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 312-318.

Christensen, C. (1980). Effects of taste quality and intensity on oral perception of viscosity. Perceptual Psychophysics, 28(4), 315-320.

Crisinel, A., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters, 464, 39-42.

Cuskley, C. (2013). Mappings between linguistic sound and motionPublic Journal of Semiotics, 5(1), 39-62.

Cuskley, C. and Kirby, S. (2013). Synaesthesia, cross-modality and language evolution. In: Simner, J. and Hubbard E.M. (Eds) Oxford Handbook of Synaesthesiapp. 869-907.

Cuskley, C., Simner, J. and Kirby, S. (2015). Phonological and orthographic influences in the bouba-kiki effect. Psychological Research, doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0709-2.

Davenport, R., Rogers, C., & Russel, I. (1973). Cross-modal perception in apes. Neuropsychologia, 11(1), 21-28.

Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Why we are not all synesthetes (not even weakly so). Psychonomic bulletin & review, 20(4), 643-664.

Dingemanse, M. (2011). Ideophones and the aesthetics of everyday language in a West-African society. Senses and Society, 6(1), 77-85.

Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in cognitive sciences, 19(10), 603-615.

Dowman, M. (2008). The multiple stages of protolanguage. In A. D. Smith, K. Smith, & R. Ferrer i Cancho (Eds.), The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 6th international conference (p. 419-420). World Scientific.

Fay, N., Garrod, S., Roberts, L., & Soboda, N. (2010). The interactive evolution of human communication systems. Cognitive Science, 34, 351-386.

Frishberg, N. (1975). Arbitrariness and iconicity: historical change in American Sign Language. Language, 696-719.

Garrod, S., Fay, N., Lee, J., Oberlander, J., & MacLeod, T. (2007). Foundations of representatoin: Where might graphical symbol systems come from? Cognitive Science, 31(6), 961-987.

Gasser, M. (2004). The origins of arbitrariness in language. In Proceedings of the 26th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 434-439). Hillsdale, NJ.

Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J. (1994). Introduction: Sound symbolic processes. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols, & J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (Cambridge, UK ed., p. 1-14). Cambridge University Press.

Imai, M., Li, L., Haryu, E., Okada, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (2008). Novel noun and verb learning in Chinese, English, and Japanese speaking children. Child Development, 79, 979-1000.

Imai, M., & Kita, S. (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 369(1651), 20130298.

Kohler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York, NY: Liveright.

Johansson, N., & Zlatev, J. (2013). Motivations for sound symbolism in spatial deixis: A typological study of 101 languages. Public Journal of Semiotics, 5(1), 3-20.

Kirby, S., Tamariz, M., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2015). Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition, 141, 87-102.

Ludwig, V., Adachi, I., & Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

Ludwig, V., & Simner, J. (2012). What colour does that feel? tactile– visual mapping and the development of cross-modality. Cortex(0), -. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.004

Martino, G., & Marks, L. E. (2001). Synesthesia: Strong and weak. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 61–65.

Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R. C., Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S. (2014). How arbitrary is language?. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 369(1651), 20130299.

Parise, C., Spence, C., & Deroy, O. (2015). Understanding the correspondences: introduction to the special issue on crossmodal correspondences. Multisens Res, 29, 1-6.

Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., Winter, B., Massaro, D. W., & Lupyan, G. (2017). Iconicity in the speech of children and adults. Developmental Science.

Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: from a world of experience to the experience of language. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 369(1651), 20130300.

Pietrandrea, P. (2002). Iconicity and arbitrariness in italian sign language. Sign Language Studies, 2(3), 296-321.

Ramachandran, V., & Hubbard, E. (2001). Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(1), 3-34.

Ravignani, A., & Sonnweber, R. (2017). Chimpanzees process structural isomorphisms across sensory modalities. Cognition, 161, 74-79.

Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 239-255.

Simner, J., Cuskley, C. and Kirby, S. (2010). What sound does that taste? Cross-modal mappings across gustation and audition. Perception, 39(4), 553-569.

Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E., Witherby, S. A., Fraser, C., . . . Ward, J. (2006). Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experi- ences. Perception, 35, 1024-1022.

Simner, J., Ward, J., Lanz, M., Jansari, A., Noonan, K., Glover, L., & Oakley, D. A. (2005). Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(8), 1069-1085.

Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 73(4), 971-995.

Tamariz, M., Roberts, S. G., Martínez, J. I., & Santiago, J. (2017). The Interactive Origin of Iconicity. Cognitive Science.

Theisen, C. A., Oberlander, J., & Kirby, S. (2010). Systematicity and arbitrariness in novel communication systems. Interaction Studies, 11(1), 14-32.

Thompson, P., & Estes, Z. (2011). Sound symbolic naming of novel objects is a graded function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(12), 2932-2404.

Verhoef, T., Kirby, S., & Boer, B. (2016). Iconicity and the emergence of combinatorial structure in language. Cognitive science, 40(8), 1969-1994.

Ward, J., Huckstep, B., & Tsakanikos, E. (2006). Sound-colour synaesthesia: to what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all? Cortex, 42, 264-280.

Werner, H. (1957). Comparative psychology of mental development (revised edition). New York: International Universities Press.

Witthoft, N., & Winawer, J. (2006). Synesthetic colors determined by having colored refrigerator magnets in childhood. Cortex, 42(2), 175-183.

Yoshida, H. (2012). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism in children’s verb learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(2), 232-265.

Advertisements